MEXICO AND SWINE FLU: an imaginary line between fear and discrimination
Prof. Korstanje Maximiliano E.
Departamento de Ciencias Económicas.
Universidad de Palermo, Argentina
Industrial civilization, generally, constitute a
favorable mean to the development of mental insanity. If the progress of Science reduces
the limits of errors, as a consequence it encourages the obsession for knowledge; the life
at office, abstract speculations and the eternal agitation of spirit
inasmuch as
complex and hard a discipline may be, more likelihood and risk to drive towards the madness (Foucault, 1998:
48-49).
Abstract
After a sudden outbreak of Swine flu appeared
in last weeks, huge attention of the globe has been brought to Mexico wherein the virus
originally emerged. As a result of this, panic to a potential pandemic struck to public
opinion in the World. In an immediate way followed to a supposed death of 150 persons, the
government of Argentina, China, Peru and Cuba ordered to their airport to suspend the
flights coming from Mexico. Visually, not only paranoia get settled in popular wisdom, but
also news papers disseminate once and once again in their different unedited editions
columns based on troublesome effects of this virus reinforcing the panic-flight behavior
in readers. Our findings back that in contexts of uncertainness people recur to
stereotypes and discriminative practices to alleviate the anxiety that is present in the
core of society. Ethnocentrism is the appropriate term to denote our conduct whenever the
horizon is unclear.
Key Words: Swine Flu Paranoia
Fear Mexico - Discrimination
Introduction
At the end of April of 2009, the world
brought its attention to Mexico wherein almost 150 people initially died because a
mutation of Swine Influenza. In spite of economical damages these types of news caused to
exportation and tourism, Health World Organization declared an emergency throughout the
globe. Panic surfaced in the core of industrialized societies was immediately disseminated
to the peripherical third World. Under such a context, the present exploratory paper is an
attempt to describe qualitatively how fears worked in people during a lapse of
uncertainness in Argentina during the months of April to July respectively. For that, we
will recur to an analysis content of a well-known virtual room wherein chatters talk about
diverse matters. This viewpoint will be
enriched contrasting with journalist sources and columns published in most important
American as well as Argentinean newspapers.
Even though specialized literature has
emphasized that Mass Media plays a pivotal role in dissemination of information in moments
of disaster (Hovland, 1953) (Freidson, 1953) (Klapper, 1963) (Wenger and Friedman, 1986)
(Quarantelli, 1982) (Tierney, 1994) (Quarantelli, 1990) (Dahlhamer and Nigg, 1994) (Paul
et al, 2003) (Beck, 2006) (Mileti, 1999) (Nigg, 1995) (Rodriguez, Diaz and Aguirre, 2004),
journalism seems to enhance fear and excitement in non-professional people by means of
distorting fundamental information (Fritz and Mathewson, 1957) (Dynes, 1972). The
time-frame of this research is ranging from the end of April to beginning of June of 2009.
Statically speaking, hitherto World Health Organization recognized almost 35.958 infected
and 163 lethal cases, in countries such as United States (17.855), México (6.403), Canada
(4.049), United Kingdom (1.461), Chile (2.335) and Argentina (733). Of course, the growing and expanding of this virus
seems to be imminent. Underpinned in the proposition that Mass media multiplies
voluntarily or not effects of Swine flu on consumers, the present research explores how
hazard over our children and lived ones reproduce and disseminate panic throughout the
population. In brief, it seems as though
people feel an extreme fear whenever their next generation is somehow jeopardized. To wit,
mythical past is retrieved as a form of coming across with problems in uncertain
circumstances.
Of course, it is strongly important to
remember that communication is a two-way process where audience receive and elaborate the
message in a proactive manner. Basically, the presence of a disaster social memory in
popular wisdom and journalism seems to be unquestionable. Ultimately, whenever a situation
of uncertainness is accompanied by the dissemination of non-edited news which helps
creating confusion and misinformation. Subsequently, a third facet comes on stage at the
time State and Mass Media articulate and combine efforts in creating an image of reality
with emphasis on discrimination and exclusion to the otherness which is perceived as
infected. To what extent racism and xenophobia will range from subtle to extreme because
of disaster appearance seems to be unresolved and no attention was given in recent
literature about disasters managing. Under a situation of disaster, otherness is often
utilized as an instrument of alleviating the tension between the life and territory. Once
again, whatever is generally implicit in moments of disasters is the fear of dying and the
exclusion of other helps us alleviating such a burden.
Half-Century
of Disaster investigation in Social Science
Centered on a historical background by
respecting to the role played by Disaster Research Center from 1950 to today, Mcneil and
Quarantelli introduce a brief summary of most important research in disaster fields. This
article is aimed at discussing the contributions and limitations found in
interdisciplinary studies of disaster. As individual entities, human beings are
circumscribed to the life in society. Disasters whatever their nature may be, affect
communities and change their perspective along with a future similar event. Macneil and
Quarantelli classify their review in five important parts: a) community disasters, b)
individual behavior, c) organizational behavior, d) community behavior, and e) mass
communication systems.
It is important to mention that living in a
society collective conduct will depend on many factors. Even though disaster impacts
cannot be differentiated according to their nature as technological or natural, every day
emergency organizations devote their attention in outstanding situations of instability
wherein many players are immediately associated with policies of rescue. Sometimes,
disasters are more impact and conflictive whenever they are unexpected. Day-to-day
emergencies look to be incorporated in the routine of social life while affected victims
can recur to a broader net in search of support. In contrast, in cases of earthquakes,
flood and other catastrophes assistance are relatively restricted. An alternative
hypothesis of research points out mass media distribution degree of news is proportional
to a reduction of certainness and forewarning.
Following this line of reasoning, many
investigators agree that in some post-impact situations most of victims show
characteristic related to irritability, anxiety or sleeplessness. For other hand, western
societies are characterized by certain tendency to apply primary knowledge in the
prevention of potential disasters (McNeil and Quarantelli, 2008). However, after further
discussion a new question has been raised: ¿what are the basis for panic emergence in
industrialized and non-industrialized societies?.
In the threshold of a new millennium
characterized by the resurgence of natural disasters, floods, and hurricanes product of
global warming as well as the industrialized economies oversights, a growing number of
researchers have concerned about the social implications and consequences of climate
disasters on our day-to-day life. Under such a context, the Journal hosted by the
prestigious Cristobal Colon University in Mexico published in 2007 a special issue fully
dedicated to public concerns on social risk implications and vulnerability and genre in
humanitarian and natural disasters. It is important to discuss critically to what an
extent natural disasters reinforces socially previous economic imbalances in local
communities at the same time focuses on how to prevent unexpected economic effects of
vulnerability in potential victims. A situation of this nature is broadly wide-spread in
the so-called Third World from Sri Lanka to Argentina wherein a major part of population
finds their basic needs unsatisfied or live under the line of poverty.
As the previous argument given, F.
Briones-Gamboa introduces readers as to how scholars developed the concepts of risk, fear
and anxiety in the line of time. Basically, even if there is no consensus about the
historical roots of Risk, it is often assumed this term stemmed from resecum, a Latin word meant what hurts
while others trace its origin in the word rizha that
connotes to threats a person may experience alongside river navigation. Enrooted in the
belief that risk is often associated to future and uncertainness feeling, Briones-Gamboa
argues that there are two kinds of risks: natural and social. However, it was no before
than XVII that the term fear gave rise to the development of risk and security. That
follows, it is important to note that the concept of risk corresponded with threats of
catastrophes and disasters effects. To be exact, on 1 of November in 1772 one of strongest
earthquakes in history of Europe produced 100.000 fatal victims in Portugal. As a result
of this, eternal laws of Christianism questioned and philosophers entered in a deep-seated
debate as to what extent Gods omnipotence was real or why God leaves these
unfortunate events happen. Not only the Tragedy of Lisbon allowed humanity to adopt other
ways to anticipate on this type of disasters, but also paved the way gradually for the
emergence of modern Science. From that event onwards, risk as an object of research has
received considerable attention in classical literature from many scholars and
Scientifics. The upshot of Briones-Gamboa is that notions of risk, threat, security, fear
or anguish are associated to the diverse perception of humanity to understand their own
environment as well as preventing the worse consequences of natural disasters
(Briones-Gamboa, 2007)
Discussion
social conditions for panic.
The fact is that Panic is for E. Quarantelli,
like crime in Durkheim, part of the society. Of course, some disaster scholars have thought that a more
sociological approach might parallel what Durkheim, a major figure in the origin of
sociology, said about crime. If it did not exist, it would be necessary for human
societies to create crime, at least symbolically, so as to emphasize the fact that human
beings generally adhere to social norms. So perhaps, the idea of the possibility of panic
is necessary in society to highlight the fact that human being in contrast react
remarkably well in most stressful situations and that the social bonds between and among
people usually holds (Quarantelli, 2001: 9).
For other hand, it is necessary not to forget
that in U. Beck, threats are implicitly derived as a product of the economic development
where society lies, but sooner or later a situation of increasing dangers is too much for
the ability of response either society can tolerate, and system definitely collapses. In
opposition to bourgeois society, which maintained the line between poverty and richness,
modern societies face a new configuration of social order. The fact is that this
afore-mentioned shift has been given birth to a new kind of society known as The
Society of Risk wherein fears and risk are indiscriminately distributed to all
strata of community. Needs of mass-consumption in association with a growing sentiment of
fear paved the pathway to the advent of a new spirit of capitalism. After further examination, Beck explains that in
traditional society of class, groups configure their distinctions according to the style
of consumption embodied in early socialization processes. Risks are daily experienced
individually or internally beyond the possibilities of peoples response. On hands of
journalism or biology authority, risks not only appear to be multiplied in last years
everywhere, but also have declined the cognitive sovereignty of citizens. Ultimately, by
alleviating the burden caused by perception of dangers, the market offers to consumers a
set of alternative products with the end of alleviating the tension (Beck, 2006).
An interesting recent research conducted by
Rodriguez, Diaz and Aguirre found whether people perceive that a high risk of being
injured by an event, we would expect them to develop initiatives that will result in an
endeavor of reducing possibilities to die; nevertheless, if persons does not perceive
risks for themselves or their lived ones no appropriate course of actions will be taken.
Taking their cue from Slovic et al researchers argue that ethnic minorities not only are
more vulnerable in comparison with Anglo-Saxon counterparts, but also are less likely to
follow recommended actions in cases of urgencies or disasters; even if officials invest an
increasing amount of money in warning communicating campaigns, sometimes an inadequate
communication can wreak havoc producing crises of a broader and deeper impact in
population. The question as to whether race correlates with indifference to preparedness
predisposition should be put under scrutiny because of ethnocentric. However, author are
convinced that the adoption of a recommended
action also appears to be correlated with ethnicity and race, with the resulting
implication that minorities will be less likely to adopt the recommended actions in cases
of severe whether events relative to their majority counterparts. This issue is further
complicated given that, despite the fact that minorities are more likely to report higher
levels of perceived risk, they are less likely to receive the warnings that would allow
them to take protective action (Rodriguez,
Diaz and Aguirre, 2004: 13). Of course, it would be unquestionable that cultural patterns
show certain influence between disaster and social preparedness.
In turn, Waxman (1973) provided enough
evidence to affirm gate-keeping process is not being followed by reporters in moments of
panics and disorder. At that rate, radio stations not only centered all information
provided by official sources or public calls but also distributed such information without
any kind of editing. The underlying problem looks to be unedited information dissemination
can generate unexpected aftermaths as looting or crimes (Waxman, 1973).
With more similarities than difference,
Quarantelli and Wenger examine the impact of two natural disasters in United States and
Japan as well as the response of journalism in each case.
One of first things both have in common is that disasters are dealt with as important news stories at the time they happened.
For other hand, in both countries electronically media web site support is constantly
consulted in order for audience to receive information. By respecting to differences,
researchers go on to argue that altruism finds a major presence in Japanese society rather
than American. The extensive assistance given to local radio station as long as the
Tsunami disaster in Nagasaki by all other media corporation of Japan is evidently inverse
with United States wherein journalism is characterized by competition in generating the
own stories in detriment of other local stations (Quarantelli and Wenger, 1989).
Nevertheless, the fact is that coverage of
disasters or lootings differ between Television, newspapers and radio stations. An
interesting contribution of E. Quarantelli suggests that few planning in gate-keeping
process was found in lapses of instability in televised news in comparison with radio or
newspapers. In addition, reporters in American culture trend to assume certain autonomy
from mass media organizations. Even if newspapers gain more attention in population in
post-impact-period, it is truth that live coverage during disasters is common in
electronic media because of immediacy issues. That way, said Quarantelli television in particular is prone to perpetuating
disaster myths. For example, although reference to panic and looting constitutes only a
small proportion of the total television content, their presentation is very dramatic and
consistent with the mythologies (Quarantelli, 1990:7).
As an experiment conducted by Kanehman and
Tversky demonstrated many years back, effects of news will depend upon the order how they
are presented. Starting from different theoretical premises, researchers carried out an
interview wherein people were asked as follows figure
out United States is being prepared for a strange Asian disease in which case 600 persons
will die. Under such a context, two programs are affordable (A and B). Scientifics assume
that whether program A is chosen 200 person will survive; inversely, if program B is
taken, there is a third of possibilities that 600 can be salved but two thirds everyone
die. The findings seems to settle the issue that 72% of interviews opted for program A
while 28% for B. Alternatively, whether program C
is followed only 400 person will but if D is chosen, we come across with a third of
possibilities that nobody die but two thirds that everybody fall in such a
misfortune. Of course, 22% of participants inclined to accept program C while
78% program D; these answers support the idea that influence of Mass Media over audience
will depend on how is articulated and showed the news and framing (Kanheman and Tversky, 1984).
Beyond ethical boundaries of journalism, the
point of discussion, here, seems to be whether news should be edited or transmitted in
rough during a moment of uncertainness. As a whole, the debate is circumscribed to
non-editing news can result in uncontrollable consequences in moment of disasters while
edited-news very well jeopardize the freedom of press increasing the likelihood of
censorship. If we start from the premise that emergencies afford politicians to manipulate
the news, we will be accepting implicitly a future censorship. As the example of certain groups or elites, which
recur to the manipulation of fear to shaping a new locale in their benefit. Possibly, as Quarantelli argued, audience is
not so easily influential than supposed. Rather, there is no simple or direct response to a warning.
There is a reaction rather than a response. But actual or potential disaster victims do
not react in a uniform way to perceived and confirmed warning messages. The consequence is
that just as there are differentiated perceptions, there are differentiated
responses (Quarantelli, 1982: 8).
The question as to whether television tends
to perpetuate disaster myth and panic flight is reassumed by Wenger and Friedman in
comparative research published originally in 1986. Taking their cue from Goltz, these
scholars argued that there is hard evidence in which case popular disaster myths are very
well disseminated by visual mass-media; nonetheless, in contrasting with Goltz, they are
strongly convinced that print media allows perpetuating such a myth in social imaginary
and reinforce a collective memory of disaster. Methodologically, anyway it is very
difficult to determine to what an extent the coverage of journalism can graphically
reinforce stereotypes linked to panic. From this turn of mind, researchers recommend that
a quantitative analysis alone cannot answer
this question. One front-page story with accompanying pictures and graphics may create a
powerful image for the reader that reinforces mythical stereotypes, even if they are no
mentioned in thousand of additional column inches (Wenger and Friedman, 1986:
48).
However, Quarantelli in an extensive and
well-documented paper emphasized on human
behavior in circumstances of stress or disaster are not as irrational, emotional and
uncontrolled as shown in social science literature. Even if general belief is enrooted in
that people in disturbs take unimagined pathways, empirical studies support a contrasting
idea which emphasizes physical flight is not easily observable as social imaginary
supposed. Our American scholar defines panic as an acute fear reaction marked by flight behavior.
Subjectively, there is an intense fear reaction, a strong impulse to flee from a
threatening danger. Panic participants are seized by fear of a specific object perceived
as involving an immediate and extreme threat to physical survival. Overtly, the flight
behavior always involves an attempt to remove ones self physically from the
endangered area (Quarantelli, 1975: 16).
With this in mind, panic represents an
infrequent pattern of conduct and only is emerged whenever certain conditions are given
such as: a) a potential perceived threat is projected in general terms with long-run
perspectives, which may jeopardize individual capacity of surviving; of course, this may
be the case of shocking events as hurricanes or tornadoes; b) another similar event is
expected just after occurred the first one that predispose the self to withdrawal; c)
panicky reactions will happen whenever no real hazards are perceived as possible and d) present perceptions of possible entrapment are
associated to a feeling of psychological isolation as well as impotency in situations
(Quarantelli, 1975).
Ultimately, in a recent study of Dengue in
Argentina Korstanje classified three stages under epidemic outbreaks: Uncertainness,
gossip and Ideology. At a first
instance, we come across with a phase linked to uncertainness
where population is not familiar with the situation. A couple of days later, this
stage is accompanied with a lapse of gossip where
predominates fantasy, informal commentaries, and rumour that refers new cases of
infections that involve familiars, friends, colleagues, or co-workers. Ultimately, a third
ideological phase is characterized by the
transmittal of concrete information articulated by political power and mass media
corporations. However, political aspects configure the basis for construction of mythical
archetype which helps people to understand further regarding such a reality before to
their eyes. To the best of our knowledge, Mass media plays a pivotal role in these
circumstances health-scaring dangers with some giving extensive attention, even though
their ways should be put under scrutiny. (Korstanje, 2009).
Methodological
implication of qualitative research under debate
After further examination, we temporarily
conclude a multi-facet and complex phenomenon as panic should be investigated recurring to
qualitative and situational sources. Unfortunately, Social Sciences are today enrooted in
the incorrect belief that correlative economic indicators or quantitative methodologies
are more fruitful than qualitative ones. Most likely this idea can be applicable for some
specific issues but not for research in emotional-related topics. For that reason, we opted to follow the
contributions of content analysis in a sample of virtual chatters (yahoo forums) who
looked concerned by respecting to the advent of swine flu in México and elsewhere. In
addition, they were native Spanish-speakers ranging from 20 to 34 years old.
Complementarily, we will analyze the discourse of national and international newspapers as
to know how they cover this episode.
Yahoo
Virtual Chatter community
In last years, technologies in communication
have been transformed how people is being connected. Internet accompanied with more recent
software implementation facilitates the familiar connections but shift classical manners
of talking with friends or family. The methodological problem in working with these types
of sample is their impersonality. For other hand, possibilities of exaggeration in answer
are pretty high. Users of these technologies can chat with false names and references.
Besides, their responses are for some scholars of questionable validity. Beyond these
critics, anonym speech can very well became in valid information for scientific purposes
research. Controversial matters which defy social conventions should be studied following
affordable web-users communities.
That way, chatters appear to feel certain
freedom to express taboo affairs[1]. For that reason, we strongly
believe contents analysis on chatter community should be used by social Scientifics in
their study of risk perception issues. This recorded conversation was extracted from Yahoo
Forum on May 02 of 2009, just upon disseminated the news about Swine flu, a term given by
Americans from 1976 to Swine influenza. The question of this room was ¿Is swine flue
lethal for human beings?.
The first respondents to this question
expressed I read many newspapers and watched
on TV that Swine flu is lethal pandemic in almost cases. For the moment this has no cure
for what an infection is the end of all us. Furthermore, It is truth Mass media are
inclined to create panic in population (Anna). On contrary, other virtual reader
wrote what Anna said it is not real, I have
already heard that if this illness is detected in time a cure is possible.
One might think that messages involved in an
apocalyptic discourse are implicitly associated to dissemination of fears. This is a
result of social imaginary and fictional films which insisted that someday an
easily-transmittal virus will exterminate whole part of global population. Anthropologically speaking, the advent of
apocalypses is not absolute and is circumscribed in religious roots. Few people are
generally salved by intervention of outstanding divine forces. Unless otherwise resolved,
end of the world as well as dooms-day are related to the logic of work specialization.
Eschatology remains certain resemblance with the cut of harvest and division of work
(Eliade, 2006). As in many other cultures of the World, whereas cosmology existed and was
created, one day corruption of customs in humankind will determine a radical end. This
belief is no other thing than an emulation of renovation of harvests and resulted tension
between work and resting. That looks to be the role that prevention plays in these types
of circumstances. Metaphorically speaking, as Moses warned to Israels folk before
than exodus that painted the door of their households with blood of lamb, prevention is a
ritual process which continues with our basic needs of transcendence.
Another user, el sapiente que
sabe said the mortality depends on the
classification of virus which scientific name is puercum porcinum. Symptoms of this range
from a simple unease until paranoiac hallucinations
for further information consult
to the neighbor. Following this funny argument, Ranita reports Swine Influenza is not lethal, this is just a kind of
flu. Whatever it is mortal is ignorance of some people who incur in medicines without the
prescription of doctor. Other
testimonies are aimed at emphasizing that the mortality of this new virus is associated
with the immediacy of a rapid detection and treatment.
The main concern in involved chatters is the
question as to whether swine flu is or not mortal. Of course, unlike other viruses as AIDS
or Ebola-Zaire, classical flu is of easily infection but low mortality. In fact, people
pass flu on to the other by means of personal contact, in a conversation or sharing
utensils. In Ramireza for example, her argument moves toward other direction,
Swine flu is a mix of human and, avian and
pork. Of course, mortality is high if people do not follow the indication of Health
officials. If within 3 days you wont realize that you are infected surely you will
die. I hope this information helps. Similar
thought was found in Hopeth who argued if the
swine flu is attended afterwards 48 hours of emergence of symptoms, you can´t be salved
.
Needless to say, that almost all users have
incorrect their information about the effects of this new troublesome virus. After further
examination, in 1.119 words and 6.2006 characters, extreme terms such as die, lethal or mortal are present once and once again in
several paragraphs (15 times), followed in secondary position of Swine (7), in time (5), virus (3), doctor
(1), hospital (1) verbs as to detect or to find (1). For other hand, in case of
Pakko impacts look to be related to the number of deceased people in Mexico
which increases roughly to 100. Mysteriously, no message discriminating Mexicans are shown
in this room. However, the degree of misinformation about the forms of contagion as well
as consequences of Swine influenza is evident. All collected answers encompass a stage of
uncertainness wherein information is unclear and fuzzy; the role played by Mass media
corporations as channels for disseminating appropriate information is not only utopist but
also converse with their real interests at stake (Korstanje, 2009). The observed huge fear
of death still should be investigated; as it will be examined in next lines, this
underlying fear is transmitted by graphic press in newspapers.
Newspapers
and pandemic.
Basically, the term pandemic is used to
denote an epidemic of infectious disease. However, the problem lies in not all of them
cause the death or exterminate a whole population as modern films, stories, fiction
sciences best-seller books or cartoons show. Like many other institutions fantasy
contributes popular wisdom to depict its own reality. Of course, this is the case of
classical influenza which is widely spread throughout the world and is inoffensive for an
uttermost part of people. For other hand, it is clear that pan-demic comprises a similar
ethimological composition than panic: pan and demic from demos (folk). The Greek suffix
pan was used to symbolize the God of shepherds principally venerated in Archadia. As many
other gods, Pan characterized by an ambiguous nature; half human and goat, he represented
at the same time contradictory sentiments such as brutality and amusement.
In brief, this figure inspires a sudden fear
in lonely places because his reactions might be unforeseeable (Dupuy, 1999). It is often hypothesized that the work-frame of
mass media coverage is based on whatever social imaginary requires. Under such a context,
it is strongly necessary to catch a glimpse of the role played by Mass media in
disseminations of news. The point seems to be: ¿are mass media news aimed at generating
panic flights?, ¿how easily influenced may be an audience to the exposition of
tragedy-related news?. Of course, interests on these questions persisted over many years.
Sometimes, following certain individual interests, political power imposes risks upon
societies univocally; different segment of risks are allocated in society by means of Mass
media. As Tierney (1994) has been examined, it is not clear yet the relationship between
risks perception and power; this is what is originally missing from the dominant
scientific perspective in the study of risk today (Tierney, 1994).
On April 28, the American well-known
newspapers New York Times headlined The new Swine Flu regarding the outbreak
supposedly-originated in Mexico which affected more than 500 inside and beyond such a
country. One of more impacting lines of this column were written in form of questions:
Is the new swine flu virus that has killed
many people in Mexico and has spread to the United States and other countries the start of
a much feared pandemic?. Or is this yet another false alarm the latest in a long
history of worrying that some day a hugely lethal flu strain might sweep through the World
and kill tens of millions of people, much as it did in 1918-1919?.[2]
Back
in 1918, when the deadly Spanish influenza hit New York, the city had more than a few
disadvantages, compared with the current situation. Overcrowding was rampant, the course
of disease unclear. The Malady is being fought principally with sunshine and fresh air,
said one article in the New York Times soon after it reported the illness struck in late
summer [3].
In counterpart, a picture in black and white shows how worked street cleaners as long
as Spanish influenza in 1918.
Picture
1 Street Cleaners
Source: New York Times.
Whether we imagine this for a moment, this
message condenses an extreme and shocking impact on readers. As we have already mentioned,
social memory or imaginary has got yet in mind the consequences of Spanish Influenza in
1918-1919 that killed many million of people. This mythical archetypes is also reproduced
in this analyzed new as a form of enhancing panic. For one hand, it is emphasized on one
question; would have certain resemblance this unknown swine flu with others lethal
pandemics?.
For the other, the presence of death is set
up as a mechanism to create indoctrination, identity and affiliation. As a result of this,
binomial logic of United States / Mexico, we and they, are camouflaged in the paragraph
that has killed many people in Mexico and has
spread to the United States. Can this virus kill American as well?. Of course,
certain spirit of superiority and ethnocentrism still remain and these types of outbreaks
intend to potentiate certain stereotypes and prejudices. This point leads us to the second
elements presents in mass media in relation to our analysis: the protection of our children. The lamentable
death of one baby inside American soil was covered by the British newspapers The
Independent as follows: US baby dies as new
Spanish case raises swine flu alarm. An
US government official said a 23-month-old child in southern state had died from the
virus. A health official said the baby was a resident of Mexico [4].
A distinction of this babys nationality or residence is unimportant, however,
emphasis on the residency of victim allow alleviating the excitement in American
imaginary.
Similar treatment received a death of a baby
3 month old in Argentina on June 16. However, in this case the source of contagion still
remains uncertain. In this case, authorities are disconcerted by respecting to where she
can be infected. This column emphasizes once and once again the different pulmonary
problems this baby had in view of the premature condition of her birth. Of course, her
family had never outside Argentina in which case experts assume H1N1 is being circulated
among population. As a result of this, many schools were closed in Buenos Aires and
outskirts[5].
Many mileage far-away, other claims of
discrimination in a column published Clarin dated
on 06 May of 2009 impacted in argentine public opinion; emulating on supposed words of
president of Mexico who asked to entire World to leave restricted measures in which
case are involved Argentina, Peru, Ecuador and China- that prevented landing of flights
from Mexico and danger seriously the economy of such a country, Silvina Heguy a reporter
of Clarin emphasized that in Face-book and virtual Forums started to circulating messages
against to argentines[6].
On the same edition but in page 20, Carolina Burstein collects a bunch of statements of
Mexicans with residency in Buenos Aires who stated having experienced a subtle
discrimination because of swine flu dissemination[7]. However, at the same time
our reporter reminds in street of Buenos Aires Mexican received daily support from people.
Quite aside from this, Health official of Texas spokesman confirmed that a woman of 30
years old died last week portrayed the H1N1 virus. With emphasis on the number of infected
people in United States, this newspapers focus on an email sent by W. Gheen, president in
ALIPAC Americans for illegal migration- to the Press wherein he accused to Barak
Obama president of United States- as illogic; let reader to remind B. Obama
qualified Gheen´s words as racists as well as the turn of mind of all who wishing to have
boundaries closed so that illegal Mexicans cannot spread this pandemic to the core of
Unites States-[8].
Partially, our finding support an earlier
mentioned research of Korstanje who specified three different stages in moments of
epidemic related to: uncertainness wherein people have ambivalent news and a broader
horizon in decision making process; gossip allows reducing where the gap between reality
and unclear news enlarge. Ultimately, ideology permits to articulate political instrument
by manipulating news and create a new circle of legitimacy, discrimination and identity. A
re-restructuration of identity is a common observable issue in these contexts. We say
partially because this issue should continue being investigated under many other
perspectives. Whatever the case may be, other
media-coverage, for examples those of La Nacion newspapers headlined on the top cover,
Argentine government cancel polemically all
flights to Mexico. In view of these lines, this column focuses on Ministry of
Health reports which said that no cases of Swine flu were found in Argentina.
Ironically, this article criticizes the most
recent one-way decided measures of Government regarding policies of outbreak prevention
whereas -for other hand- such an argument is very well reinforced by a combination of
disturbing pictures wherein five employers of a well-known airplane company are on desk
wearing a mouth-guard at Buenos Aires Airport. This image, obviously, contrasts with early
argument that readers can follow in this release[9]. As shown below in other
pictures, tourists are staged transiting with mouth-guards and their luggage. Because of
exposition purposes we only limit to analyze a few of many pictures published in
newspapers of Europe, Latin America and United States.
Picture
2 Tourists at Barajas, Airport of Madrid.
Source: La Nación. 27April of 2009
As shown above, it is interesting that the
shot of picture has been taken downwards to highlight serious impacts of Swine Flu in
public opinion. These two travellers are sworn with mouth-guard and certain facial
expression of concern. This elderly couple was chosen as a form of emulating panic in
population. Unlike youth who are less vulnerable to this epidemic, more vulnerable
children and elderly men or women represent a clear target of this epidemic. Colours in the ladys scarf allow capturing
the attention of readers as well. This
picture was taken and published before than an important account of infected people
increased significantly in United States, Spain, France, United States and other part of
Europe.
Conclusion.
As stated on introductory section, we have so
far examined the specialized literature by respecting to the performance of Mass Media in
contexts of fear, panic flights and disaster contrasting their pivotal points to how react
a community of virtual chatters in Yahoo Forum. Next, findings were contrasted to existing
published news papers such as New York Times in United States and Clarin in Argentina
during the time-frame of April, May and July. Of course, a discussion of this nature not
only is strictly circumscribed to this work but also will continue in next years. Like
Dengue in Argentine, Swine flu is shocking the core of United States and Europe in these
moments. In perspective, the performance of Corporations in the coverage of this epidemic
will be pivotal in next hours. The discussion is around an ethical tension between the
freedom of Press and responsibility in disseminating their news. Whatever the result may
be, it is strongly important to remember that communication is a two-way process where
audience catches and elaborates the message in a proactive-manner. Basically, the presence
of a disaster social memory in popular wisdom and journalism seems to be unquestionable.
Starting from the premise that people scares
whatever they are unable to control, we will analyze qualitatively how has impacted the
dissemination of Swine Flu mortality in popular wisdom. Backing other studies, our
outcomes reflect that in cases of health emergencies three stages are predominant
(Korstanje, 2009). To put this in brutally, a moment of uncertainness wherein people get a bit confused and
receive ambivalent information by respecting to the consequences of certain event; this
stage is accompanied by a second one characterized by the hegemony of gossip in which case cognitive dissonance is
temporarily reduced. In relation to Swine flu, virtual chatters discuss regarding the
wide-range of mortality of such a virus handling inappropriate body of knowledge.
As a chatter who ironically declared for further information consult to the
neighbor, in these circumstances jokes, irony and gossip play a pivotal role in
alleviating the social anxiety players feel in this game. Ultimately, mass media
articulates symbolically a construct to show what they infer as the reality;
of course this last stage is linked to ideology.
In a related vein, depictions of certain events are framed following a circle of
discrimination and exclusion whose criterion range from national to ethnic affiliation.
Sometimes, political power can expiate their own faults (as authorities) utilizing these
discourses against minorities. As posed in this paper, racism and discriminatory practices
may emerge only whether ideology identities potential reasons of anxiety in ethnic
minorities. With this background in mind, migrants and outlanders -or any group considered
at the bottom of hierarchal order- are usually accused as responsible of bringing viruses
or strange illness because of conditions wherein they live. From other perspective,
disaster perception risk reinforces preexisting historical boundaries by means of
imposition of stereotypes, prejudice behaviour, racism or discrimination of other broader
nature; for example a semantic opposition between American / Mexican has been present in
whole part of analyzed press in United States and UK. For an instance, as Mexicans living
in Buenos Aires argued, their country has been stereotyped in social imaginary as the
cradle of Swine flu. As a whole, the present research supports a previous speech of
Durkheim and Quarantelli who emphasized that in disaster situations societies recur to a
social memory of similar events to configure next courses of actions. In other terms,
Swine flu outbreak in 2009 remitted to Spanish Influenza in 1918-1919 even if contextual
circumstances of both vary on. Thus, it is
important to denote that disaster myths are perpetuated by journalisms intervention
(Wenger and Friedman, 1986) (Quarantelli, 1982) (Tierney, 1994) (Quarantelli, 1990).
References
Beck,
U. (2006). The Society of Risk: towards a new
modernity. Barcelona, Paidos.
Briones-Gamboa, F.
(2007). The complexity of risk. Journal
of Cristobal Colon´s University. Issue 20 (3): 9-19.
Dahlhamer, J. and
Nigg, J. (1994). An Empirical investigation of rumouring: anticipating disaster
under conditions of uncertainty. Disaster Research
Center, Preliminary Paper 216
Dynes, R. (1970).
Organized behavior on disasters. Columbus,
Disaster Research Center, Ohio University.
Dupuy, J. P.
(1999). The Panic. Barcelona, Gedisa.
Eliade, M.
(2006). The myth of eternal Return. Buenos
Aires, Emece.
Freidson,
E. (1953). Communication Research and the concept of the mass. American
Sociological Review. Vol. 18 (3): 313-317.
Fritz,
C. E. and Mathewson, J (1957). Convergence
Behaviour in Disaster. Study No. 9. Washington D.C, National Academy of
Sciences.
Foucault,
M. (1998). History of Madness in classical
Epoch. México, Fondo de Cultura Económica.
Hovland,
C. (1953). Communication and Persuasion,
psychological studies of opinion change. New Haven, Yale University Press.
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1984). Choices,
Values and frames. American Phsychologist
39 (4): 341-350.
Klapper,
J. T. (1963). The Science of human
communication. Nueva York, Basic Books.
Korstanje,
M. (2009). Mass media performance in the construction of gossip: the case of
Dengue in Argentina 2009. Unpublished article in bias of assessment.
McNeil,
S. and Quarantelli, H. E. (2008). Past, present and Future: building an
interdisciplinary disaster research center on Half-century of Social Science Disaster
Research. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary
Paper, 362.
Mileti,
D. S. (1999). Disaster by Design: a
Reassessment of Natural Hazard in the United States. Washington D.C, Joseph. Henry
Press.
Nigg,
J. M. (1995). Risk Communication and Warning System. Pp. 369-382 in T.
Horlick-Jones, A. Amdeola, and R. Casale (Eds.), Natural
Risk and civil Protection. London, E. & F.N. Spon.
Paul,
B. K et al. (2003). Public Response to Tornado Warnings: a comparative Study of
the May 04, 2003 Tornadoes in Kansas, Missouri and Tennessee. Quick Response
Research Report, no 165, Natural Hazard Center, University of Colorado
Quarantelli,
E. L. (1975). Panic Behavior: some empirical observations. Paper presented at the American Institute of Architects
Conference on Human Response to Tall Building, July 19, 1975, Chicago, Illinois. Available at Disaster Research Center, Preliminary
Paper, 20.
Quarantelli,
E. L. (1982). People´s reactions to emergency warnings. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 75.
Quarantelli,
E. L. (1990). The Mass media in disasters in the United States. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 150.
Quarantelli,
E. L. (2001). The Sociology of Panic. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 283.
Quarantelli,
E. L and Wenger, D. (1989). A cross Societal comparison of Disaster News
Reporting In Japan and the United States. Disaster
Research Center, Preliminary Paper 142.
Rodriguez,
H; Díaz, W. and Aguirre, B. (2004). Comunicating Risk and warnings: an integrated and
interdisciplinary Research Approach. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 337.
Tierney
J, K. (1994). Sociology´s Unique contributions to the study of Risk. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 204.
Wenger,
D. and Friedman, B. (1986). Local and National Media coverage of disaster: a
content analysis of the Print media´s treatment of disaster myths. Disaster Research Center, Preliminary Paper 185a.
[2] Editorial, New York Times. The New
Swine Flu. April 27 of 2009. Available at http://www.nytimes.com/2009/04/28/opinion/28tue1.
Extracted on May 06.
[3] Big City, New York Times. In 1918 flu
outbreak, a cool head prevailed. April 30 of 2009. Available at http://www.nytimes.com/2009/05/01/nyregion/01bigcity.html.
[4] Health and Wellbeing. The Independent. 29
April of 2009. Available at http://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-wellbeing/health-news/texas-baby-first-flu-death-reported-outside-mexico-1676007.html.
Extracted on 06 may of 2006
[5] Society: a baby 3 month old is
the first victim of A flu in Argentina. Clarin electronic edition. 16 June of 2009.
Available at http://www.clarin.com/diario/2009/06/16/sociedad/s-01939960.htm,
[6] The World, Calderon bade for reducing
the restrictive measures against Mexico. 06 May of 2009. Year LXIV, issue 22.757.
Page 18. By Silvina Heguy.
[7] Global Warning, The Mexicans in Buenos
Aires, between solidarity and suspiction. 06 May of 2009. Year LXIV, issue 22.757.
Page 20. By Carolina Brunstein.
[8] Global Warning, A second victim
inside United States dies victim of Swine Flu. 06 May of 2009. Year LXIV, issue
22.757. Page 19.
[9] Risk of Pandemic. Argentine government suspends polemically all flights
to Mexico. 29 April of 2009. Year 140, No. 49.426.